Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://doi.org/10.21256/zhaw-1446
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorStocker, Christa-
dc.date.accessioned2018-09-18T11:59:50Z-
dc.date.available2018-09-18T11:59:50Z-
dc.date.issued2000-
dc.identifier.issn1023-2044de_CH
dc.identifier.urihttps://digitalcollection.zhaw.ch/handle/11475/10722-
dc.description.abstractThe studies by Braun et al. (1998), Klein (1988) and Khosroshahi (1989) show that masculine generics (appellatives and pronouns) tend to trigger associations of males rather than of males and females and even the use of gender splittings and neutral generic pronouns (he or she, she or he, they) respectively does not inevitably lead to an appropriate mental representation of female and male referents. The fact that women often remain mentally underrepresented in gender neutral situations, even though the personal reference is carried out through gender splitting forms, depends on the mechanisms of categorization, on the fact that most situations are gendered, and on a general gendercentrism. Associations recipients make therefore crucially depend on the following three factors: Firstly on the recipient’s gender, secondly on whether a situation is dominantly linked to male or female connotations, and thirdly on whether the named category has gendered category-bound features.de_CH
dc.language.isodede_CH
dc.publisherVereinigung für Angewandte Linguistik in der Schweizde_CH
dc.relation.ispartofBulletin suisse de linguistique appliquéede_CH
dc.rightsLicence according to publishing contractde_CH
dc.subjectStereotypde_CH
dc.subjectLinguistikde_CH
dc.subjectRepräsentationde_CH
dc.subjectSprachede_CH
dc.subjectKategorisierungde_CH
dc.subjectVorurteilde_CH
dc.subject.ddc401.4: Terminologie, Diskursanalyse, Pragmatikde_CH
dc.titleBeidnennung und kognitive Repräsentanzde_CH
dc.typeBeitrag in wissenschaftlicher Zeitschriftde_CH
dcterms.typeTextde_CH
zhaw.departementAngewandte Linguistikde_CH
zhaw.organisationalunitInstitut für Übersetzen und Dolmetschen (IUED)de_CH
dc.identifier.doi10.21256/zhaw-1446-
zhaw.funding.euNode_CH
zhaw.issue72de_CH
zhaw.originated.zhawNode_CH
zhaw.pages.end153de_CH
zhaw.pages.start139de_CH
zhaw.publication.statuspublishedVersionde_CH
zhaw.volume2000de_CH
zhaw.publication.reviewPeer review (Publikation)de_CH
Appears in collections:Publikationen Angewandte Linguistik

Files in This Item:
File Description SizeFormat 
2000_Stocker_Beidnennung_und_kognitive_Repräsentanz_VALS_ASLA.pdf62.19 kBAdobe PDFThumbnail
View/Open
Show simple item record
Stocker, C. (2000). Beidnennung und kognitive Repräsentanz. Bulletin suisse de linguistique appliquée, 2000(72), 139–153. https://doi.org/10.21256/zhaw-1446
Stocker, C. (2000) ‘Beidnennung und kognitive Repräsentanz’, Bulletin suisse de linguistique appliquée, 2000(72), pp. 139–153. Available at: https://doi.org/10.21256/zhaw-1446.
C. Stocker, “Beidnennung und kognitive Repräsentanz,” Bulletin suisse de linguistique appliquée, vol. 2000, no. 72, pp. 139–153, 2000, doi: 10.21256/zhaw-1446.
STOCKER, Christa, 2000. Beidnennung und kognitive Repräsentanz. Bulletin suisse de linguistique appliquée. 2000. Bd. 2000, Nr. 72, S. 139–153. DOI 10.21256/zhaw-1446
Stocker, Christa. 2000. “Beidnennung und kognitive Repräsentanz.” Bulletin suisse de linguistique appliquée 2000 (72): 139–53. https://doi.org/10.21256/zhaw-1446.
Stocker, Christa. “Beidnennung und kognitive Repräsentanz.” Bulletin suisse de linguistique appliquée, vol. 2000, no. 72, 2000, pp. 139–53, https://doi.org/10.21256/zhaw-1446.


Items in DSpace are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.